Home  |  Archive  |  AJA @ Nature  |  Online Submission  |  News & Events  |  Subscribe  |  APFA  |  Society  |  Links  |  Contact Us  |  中文版

The andropause and memory loss: is there a link between androgen decline and dementia in the aging male?

Robert S. Tan, Shou-Jin Pu

Geriatric Medicine Fellowship Program, University of Texas, Houston, United States

Asian J Androl  2001 Sep; 3: 169-174


Keywords:  andropause; memory loss; androgens; testosterone; aging; dementia
Abstract
Studies demonstrate a decline in androgens with age and this results in the andropause. The objective of this paper is to review the literature on hormonal changes that occur in the aging males and determine if there are associations between decreased testosterone, dehydroepiandrosterone (DHEA) and decreased cognitive function. Trials of androgen replacement and its impact on cognitive function will also be analyzed. Method of analysis will be by a thorough search of articles on MEDLINE, the Internet and major abstract databases. Results of the author's own research in 302 men of the association of memory loss as a symptom in the andropause will be presented. In addition, the authors open trial of testosterone replacement in hypogonadic men with Alzheimer's disease will also be presentedThe results of the author's trial will be compared with other investigators. High endogenous testosterone level predicted better performance on visual spatial tests in several studies, but not in all studies. Likewise, testosterone replacement in hypogonadic patients improved cognitive functions in some but not all studies. Testosterone has also been shown to improve cognitive function in eugonadal men. Several studies have shown that declines in DHEA may contribute to Alzheimer's disease and the results of double blind studies with DHEA replacement and its effect on cognition will also be presented. In summary, there is still no consensus that androgen replacement is beneficial in cognitive decline but this option may prove promising in some patients.
1 Introduction

The andropause can be defined as a time in the life of aging males where hormones decline including that of androgens[1]. Alternative names for the Andropause have been suggested including the Male Menopause, Androgen Deficiency in Aging Male (ADAM) and Partial Androgen Deficiency in Aging Male (PADAM)[2,3]. While we argue for more accuracy in defining this physiological process of aging, the lay press has predicted that the term that will likely stick in the minds of the public is the andropause[4].

Cognition on the other hand, is the mental process that includes language, calculation, visual-spatial abilities, memory, reasoning, learning, social skills,imagination and attention span. With age, cognitive function may remain stable or decline. In general, cognitive function that remains stable includes attention span, everyday communication skills, language skills, and simple visual perception. Cognitive function that decline includes selective attention, naming of objects, verbal fluency, complex visualspatial skills and language analysis[5] . Moreover, age-related memory changes vary depending on the type of memory. The acquisition and early retrieval of recently acquired information is diminished; long-term memory retention does not seem to change with increasing age[6,7]. The cognitive tests commonly administered are modified Mini-Mental Status Examination, the Digital Symbol subset of the WAIS-R, and the Trail B section of the Trail Making Test. Imaging with MRI and PET scans are supplementary to the clinical assessment of cognitive decline.

Memory loss, a domain of cognitive function, occurs during the andropause. This may be secondary to the effects of declining hormones. With age, the levels of hormones may remain the same, elevated or reduced. The circulating levels of hormones that decline in aging males are

  • Testosterone: total and free testosterone

  • Dehydroepiandrosterone (DHEA) and dehydroepiandrosterone sulfate (DHEAS)

  • Growth hormone and insulin-like growth factor-1 (IGF-1)

  • Other hormones including triiodothyronine, renin and aldosterone

The effect of hormones on cognition in the older adults has been investigated for several years. Hormone effects in elderly women's cognitive function have been researched in depth[8]. There have been abundant data on the benefits of estrogen replacement on cognition and mood. The effect on cognition by estrogen should be considered as longterm and modifying factors and not the cause of Alzheimer's disease. Relatively speaking, there is paucity about the link between the cognitive change and hormones in the older men. The improvement of cognition may assist older adults to remain independent and may contribute to prolong the time to institutionalization[9].
2 Is there a link between   memory loss to  low endogenous levels of testosterone in the andropause?

The level of total, bioavailable and free testosterone declines in aging male. Testosterone declines about 100 ng/dL per decade after age 50, but bioavailable testosterone and free testosterone decline far more dramatically. Bioavailable testosterone level<70 ng/dL or total testosterone level<300 ng/dL are diagnostic of male hypogonadism[10]. Male hypogonadism can lead to symptoms such as decreased libido, erectile dysfunction, fatigue, loss of energy, muscle weakness and even memory loss[11,12]. 

In a study design that was described as exploratory analysis in a population based cohort, Barrett-Connor et al studied 547 community dwelling men aged 59-89 years in Rancho Bernado in California[13]. Serum total and bioavaiable testosterone as well as estradiol levels were measured. Subsequent to the blood tests, 12 standard neuropsychological instruments were administered including two items from the Blessed Information Memory Concentration (BIMC) Test, three measures of retrieval from Buschke-Fuld Selective Reminding Test, a category fluency test, immediate and delayed recall from Visual Reproduction Test, the Mini-Mental State Examination with individual analysis of the Serial Sevens and the World Backwards components, and the Trail-Making Test Part B. In age and education adjusted analyses, men with higher levels of total and bioavailable estradiol had poorer scores on the BIMC Test and Mini Mental State Examination. Men with higher levels of bioavailable estradiol had poorer scores on the BIMC Test and the Mini Mental State Examination. Men with higher levels of bioavailable testosterone levels had better scores on the BIMC Test and the Selective Reminding Test.

In this study, there is a suggestion that in older men, low estradiol and high testosterone levels predicted better performance on several tests of cognitive function including memory. One criticism of this study was that the blood biochemical markers such as testosterone were determined up to 4 years before the neuropsychological tests were applied. Another problem with this study is that although high estradiol levels were associated with cognitive problems in older men, no mention was made of whether there was any adjustment for body weight. Obese men tend to have higher levels of estradiol as there is more conversion of testosterone in peripheral tissues. There have been no parallel studies demonstrating a higher incidence of dementia in obese older men. Moreover, the design of this study does not prove cause and effect. In support of Barrett-Connors' work, another study by Morley et al also reported that bioavailable testosterone correlated with age-correlated cognitive and physical measures[14].

3 Does replacing testosterone improve memory in the andropause?

Epidemiological studies like that above suggest a link; it will be interesting to determine the therapeutic effects of testosterone. Janowsky et al reported that an increase in spatial cognition, but no change in other cognitive domain in healthy old men after transdermal testosterone treatment for three months[15]. He also found that working memory improved in healthy old men after testosterone supplementation for one month[16]. It is to be noted that this study was performed on healthy older men with no prior history of memory loss. This study suggests that testosterone may enhance memory even in normal men. This study unfortunately has not been replicated.

Memory loss is an established symptom in the andropause according to our study of 302 men[17]. Our study on older patients describes how they perceive and understand this aging process. We performed a non-interventional, cross-sectional study to determine what male patients report as symptoms of the andropause. In particular, we wanted to ascertain if memory loss was a predominant feature. Our hypothesis was that androgens, such as testosterone were responsible for visual-spatial and memory development. As such the aging process of andropause, which is associated with declines in testosterone levels would lead to memory loss. A standardized questionnaire of 22 questions was administered to 302 outpatients of a medical center. Information on patient demographics, understanding of the andropause, and concomitant risk factors were collected.

Of the 302 patients, 71 % were above 60 years and whites predominated at 87 %. Memory loss was reported in 36 % of the patients who felt that they had experienced the andropause. It was the third most common symptom after erectile dysfunction (46 %) and general weakness (41 %). Twenty two percent of the 302 patients had a history of diabetes. Among those that reported to have undergone the andropause, diabetic patients were more likely to report memory loss (P=0.03, OR=1.9, CI=1.1-3.4). Sixty-four percent of patients reported the onset of andropause to be between 50-70 years (the median age being 50-60 years).

Our own study highlights the importance of testosterone in maintaining cognitive functions. It supports studies of testosterone replacement in men undergoing the andropause and who have concomitant dementia. This results parallel recent reports of the neuroprotective effects of estrogens in preventing dementia. We feel that diabetes is associated with memory loss in our study because of the additional insults to cognitive function of the brain secondary to ischemia.

In our pilot study with testosterone replacement in 6 demented hypogonadic men, we hypothesized that testosterone replacement in elderly hypogonadic males may improve cognition, in particular visual-spatial[18].

The pilot study design was single blind and interventional in nature. Eighteen consecutive male patients with a diagnosis of dementia had their testosterone levels measured. Five patients of 18 (28 %) were deemed biochemically hypogonadic ( 240 ng/dL). Initial MMSE ranged from 17 to 22. The Clock Drawing Test (CDT), being a good measure of visualspatial abilities was used. Normal PSA levels were essential before treatments. The baselines were repeated at 3, 6 and 9 months of treatment with intramuscular testosterone 200 mg.

Results obtained were baseline mean testosterone=126.4 ng/dL, MMSE=19.4, CD=2.2, PSA=0.98. At 9 months of treatment, testosterone levels increased in the 5 patients from a mean of 126.4 ng/dL to 341 ng/dL (P=0.11). PSA were also elevated from a mean of 0.98 to 1.37 (P=0.07). MMSE improved from a mean of 19.4 to 23.2 (P=0.02) (Figure 1), CDT also improved from 2.2 to 3.2 (P=0.03) (Figure 2). Four of the 5 patients continued treatments beyond the 9 months; one was stopped because of hypersexual behavior.

This pilot study performed in aging males suggested that testosterone could indeed improve cognition, including visual-spatial skills in mild to moderate dementia. This could be due to the modulation of neurotransmitters by testosterone. The next step would be for a randomized, placebocontrolled double blind study to confirm these preliminary findings.  

Although our study suggests that replacing testosterone does improve cognition in hypogonadic demented older men; other researchers have not found similar effects. For example, Sih et al reported no effect on memory; recall or verbal fluency tests in older hypogonadal men after testosterone replacement for 12 months. One possible reason for the failure to find such an effect was because spatial cognition was not measured in this study[19]. We on the other hand, distinctly found improvements in visual-spatial skills in our study. We are supported by the study of Cherrier et al. They also reported that testosterone treatment increase spatial verbal memory[20]. It is also possible that effects of testosterone on memory do not become apparent until after several treatments and till eugonadic levels are reached[21].

Our pilot study did not support a prolonged effect of testosterone on cognitive function, but did support the hypotheses of the link between cognition and testosterone. There may have been several confounders in our trial, but these are being ironed out in a larger clinical trial that is on going. There is thus some support on the scientific literature for the possible role of testosterone on cognition in aging male, but it is still unclear if this is the direct effect of testosterone or the conversion to estradiol. It is our suspicion that it is testosterone itself rather than estradiol as the study by BarrettConnor suggests the opposite effects of estradiol to cognitive function in older men.

4 Is there a link between memory loss to low endogenous levels of DHEA and DHEA-S in the andropause?

DHEA is a steroid secreted by the adrenal cortex. In circulation, DHEA exists both free and bound to sulfate (DHEA-S). Secretion of DHEA is controlled by adrenocorticotrophin. DHEA has been shown to have antiglucocorticoid action. Its physiological role and mechanism of action are unclear.  It has been suggested that DHEA may have protective effect on diabetes, cancer, aging and autoimmune diseases[22]. The secretion of DHEA decreases significantly with age. Age-related androgen failure has been called the adrenopause.

The observation that DHEA and DHEA-S concentration decrease markedly with age has led to the hypothesis that decline of DHEA level may contribute to age-related changes in cognition. It has been suggested that one contributing factor to the progress of Alzheimer's disease may be lack of protective effect of DHEA[23]. Sunderland et al demonstrated that there are reduced DHEA-S levels in Alzheimer's disease[24].

Carlson et al in Canada studied the relationships between DHEAS levels and memory in both male and female Alzheimer's patients. In that study, 52 patients with a mean age of 76.2 were studied. Each was assessed using the Rivermead Behavioral Memory Test and also had their DHEAS levels measured. Those with higher levels of DHEAS scored better in remembering name associated with a picture, digit span and the Mini Mental Status Examination[25]. Yanase and his colleagues in Japan found that the level of DHEA and DHEA-S levels decreased in patients with dementia either from Alzheimer's disease or cerebrovascular disease[26]. His study notably only had a small sample of 19 patients with Alzheimer's disease. Kalmijn et al in the Netherlands reported that there was an inverse, but non-significant, association between DHEA-S and cognitive decline[27]. This was based on a community study of 189 healthy subjects. Interestingly, it was found in this study that basal free cortisol was positively related to cognitive impairment.

The data taken form the Baltimore Longitudinal Aging Study of 883 community dwelling men demonstrated that the decline in endogenous DHEA-s was independent of cognitive status and cognitive decline in healthy aging men[28]. This study was longitudinal and elegantly designed with follow-up for as long as 31 years. There were biennial assessments of DHEAS and cognitive status in this study. The results of the studies are summarized in Table 1.

Table 1.  summarizing relationship of endogenous DHEAS and Cognition.

Author

Subjects

Study Design

Results

Carlson

52

cross sectional

AD patients with higher DHEAS perform better

Yanase

19

cross sectional

Lowered DHEA-S indementing diseases

Kalmijn

189

cross sectional

Inverse association DHEA-S & Cognition

Moffat

883

  longitudinal

  No relationship

5 Does DHEA replacement improve memory in the andropause?

DHEA is widely used as a health supplement. Many older adults use this steroid to retard age-related cognition as well as physical changes. Investigations of the effects of endogenous DHEA and DHEA-S on age-related cognition changes and the efficacy of DHEA supplementation in slowing cognitive decline are an important public health issue. The Baltimore Longitudinal Aging Study do not support the hypothesis that decline in endogenous DHEA-S contributes to cognitive decline in older men after 31 years follow up[28]. The study of Carlson et al also failed to provide any evidence that DHEA-S is protective against declarative memory decline with aging in healthy elderly men and women[29].

Wolf et al reported that after two weeks of DHEA replacement has no strong beneficial effect on any of the cognitive tests in healthy elderly men and women[30]. In that study, 40 men took 50mg of DHEA daily for 2 weeks, followed by a 2 weeks wash out period and a 2 weeks placebo period. In the study, women showed better cognitive performance in one of six cognitive tests (picture memory) after DHEA. However, DHEA replacement did not show any strong beneficial effect on any measured psychological or cognitive parameters. The study could be criticized because of the study design, small size and the many possible confounders in the study. In another study conducted in France with a double blind randomized fashion, 280 healthy elderly subjects were given either a placebo or DHEA. After one year, of 50mg/day, no difference in cognition was reported[31].

However, anecdotal reports from patients do support that DHEA does improve memory, at least qualitatively if not quantitively. The National Institute of Healthis very interested in finding out more about the effects of DHEA as evidenced by the grant processes. At the present moment, it can be stated that the effect of DHEA on cognition remains controversial. Further studies are essential to establish whether there are indeed cognitive benefits are from long-term exogenous supplementation of DHEA. It seems fair to say that at this point that there may be a relationship between DHEA, but there is no evidence to suggest that DHEA is an alternative to cholinesterase inhibitors in treating Alzheimer's disease. As for testosterone therapy for cognition, there is thus some support for the possible role of testosterone in Alzheimer's disease, but it is still unclear if this is the direct effect of testosterone or the conversion to estradiol. Further long-term trials are obviously needed.

References

[1] Tan RS. The andropause mystery. 1st ed. Houston: AMRED publishing; 2001. p 182.
[2] Morales A. A misnomer for a true entity. J Urol 2000; 163: 705-12.
[3] Tan RS. Re: Andropause: a misnomer for a true entity. J Urol 2000; 164: 1319.
[4] Marini R. For men in the change of life there's hope. San Antonio Express News; 2001 May 14; Life section: 1.
[5] Park D, Schwarz N. Cognitive aging: A primer. Portland: Lawrence Erbaum Associates Inc; 2000. p 142.
[6] Sherwin BB. Mild cognitive impairment: potential pharmacological treatment options. J Am Ger Soc 2000; 48: 431-41.
[7] Grady CL, Craik FIM. Changes in memory processing with age. Curr Opin Neurobiol 2000; 10: 224-31.
[8] Tan RS, Brown J. Cognition and estrogens in the elderly women. Clinical Geriatrics 1998; 6: 10-9.
[9] McDougall GJ. Cognitive interventions among older adults. Annu Rev Nur Res 1999; 17: 2119-40.
[10] Lund BC, Bever\|Stille KA, Perry PJ. Testosterone and andropause:  the feasibility of testosterone replacement therapy in elderly men. Pharmacotherapy 1999; 19: 951-6.
[11] Basaria S, Dobs AS. Risks versus benefits of testosterone therapy in elderly men. Drug  Aging 1999; 15: 131-42.
[12] Tenover JL. Experience with testosterone replacement in the elderly. Mayo Clin Proc 2000; 75 (Suppl): S77-82.
[13] Barretti-Connor E, Goodman-Gruen D, Patay B. Endogenous sex hormones and cognitive function in older men. J Clin Endocrinol Metab 1999; 84: 3681-5.
[14] Morley JE, Kaiser F, Raum WJ, Perry HM, Flood JF, Jensen J et al. Potentially predictive and manipulability of blood serum correlates of aging in the healthy human male:  progressive deceases in bioavailable testosterone, dehydroepiandrosterone sulfate, and the ratio of insulin\|like growth factor 1 to growth hormone. Proc Natl Acad Sci 1997; 94: 7537-42.
[15] Janowsky JS, Oviatt SK, Orwoll ES. Testosterone influences spatial cognition in older men. Behav Neurosci 1994; 108: 325-32.
[16] Janowsky JS, Chavez B. Sex steroids modify working memory. J Cogn Neurosci 2000; 12: 407-14.
[17] Tan RS, Philip P. Perceptions and risk factors for the andropause. Arch Androl 1999; 43: 97-103.
[18] Tan RS. Testosterone improves cognition. American Geriatrics Society Scientific Meeting; 2000 May; Washington, USA. abstract A 9.
[19] Sih R, Morley JE, Kaiser FE, Horrace M, Perry lll , Patrick P  et al. Testosterone replacement in older hypogonadal men:  a 12\|month randomized controlled trial. J Clin Endocrinol 1997; 82: 1661-7.
[20] Cherrier M, Asthana S, Plymate S. Effects of testosterone on cognition in healthy elderly men. Soc Neurosci 1998; 24: 2118.
[21] Wolf OT, Preut R, Hellhammer Dh, Kudielka BM, Schurmeyer TH, Kirschbaum C.  Testosterone and cognition in elderly men:  a single testosterone injection blocks the practice effect in Verbal Fluency, but has no effect on spatial or verbal memory. BiolPsychiatry 2000; 47:  650-4.
[22] Smith MJ, Schmidt PJ, Rubinow DR. Cognitive effects of testosterone supplementation. Am J Psychiatry 2000; 157: 307-8.
[23] Leglhuber F, Windhager E, Neubauer C, Weber J, Reisecker F, Dienstl E et al. The role of DHEA and DHEA-S in Alzheimer's disease. Am J Psychiatry 1992; 150: 1432-3.
[24] Sunderland T, Merril CR, Harrington MG, Lawlar BA, Molcan SE, Martinez R et al. Reduced plasma dehydroepiandrosterone concentrations in Alzheimer's disease. Lancet 1989;  8662: 570.
[25] Carlson LE, Sherwin BB. Relationships among cortisol (CRT), dehydroepiandrosterone-sulfate(DHEAS), and memory in a longitudinal study of healthy elderly men and women. Neurobiology of aging 1999; 20: 315-24.
[26] Yanase T, Fukahori M, Taniguchi S, Nishi Y, Sakai Y, Takayanagi R et al. Serum dehydroepiandrosterone(DHEA) and DHEA-Sulfate (DHEA-S) in Alzheimer's disease and in cerebrovascular disease. Endocri. J. 1996; 43: 116-23.
[27] Kalmijn S, Launer LJ, Stolk RP, Jong FHD, Pols HAP, Hotman A et al. A prospective study on cortisol, dehydroepiandrosterone sulfate, and cognitive function in the elderly. J Clin Endocrinol Metab 1998; 83: 3487-92.
[28] Maffat SD, Zonderman AB, Herman SM, Blackman MR, Kawas C, Resnick SM. The relationship between longitudinal declines in dehydroepiandrosterone sulfate concentrations and cognitive performance in older men. Arch Intern Med 2000; 160: 2193-8.
[29] Carlson LE, Sherwin BB, Chertkow HM. Relationships between Dehydroepiandrosterone Sulfate(DHEAS) and Cortisol (CRT) plasma levels and everyday memory in Alzheimer's Disease patients compared to healthy controls. Hormones and Behavior 1999; 35: 254-63.
[30] Wolf OT, Neumann O, Hellhammer DH, Geiben AC, Strasburger CJ, Dressendorter RA  et al. Effects of the two-week physiological dehydroepiandrosterone substitution on cognitive performance and well-being in healthy elderly women and men. J Clin Endocrinol Metab 1997; 82: 2363-7.

[31] Forette J. DHEA\|S does not improve cognitive function i
n healthy elderly. Gerontology 2001;  47 (Suppl 1):  268.

home


Correspondence to: Dr. Robert S. Tan, Geriatric Medicine Fellowship Program, University of Texas, Houston, USA.
E-mail:  robert.s.tan@uth.tmc.edu
Received 2001-07-16    Accepted 2001-08-13